Review by Stuart Ritchie

19 Oct 2014, 19:00

The high heritability of educational achievement reflects many genetically influenced traits, not just intelligence

Research: The high heritability of educational achievement reflects many genetically influenced traits, not just intelligence Authors: Eva Krapohl, Kaili Rimfeld, et al.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS) Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS)

Ok, I know that intelligence is really important for education, and that intelligence is genetic, so we can’t do anything about that. But what about hard work, social skills, and well-being? All these things lead to better school results too, and we know we can improve them!”

The problem is that this statement gets almost everything wrong. It forgets that, whereas it is true that intelligence is strongly heritable, nobody has ever claimed that it is 100 per cent genetically determined. More importantly, it forgets that characteristics such as conscientiousness, sociability and happiness are themselves genetically influenced.

This is the topic of a new paper by King’s College London geneticists Eva Krapohl, Kaili Rimfeld, and colleagues, published in the high-profile journal PNAS. They use data on more than 13,000 identical and non-identical twins from the UK-based TEDS (Twins Early Development Study) sample, comparing data from both types of twin to calculate the proportions of genetic and environmental influence on behaviours and traits.

Starting with the observation that educational achievement is heritable — in TEDS, genetic differences explain 62 per cent of the variation in GCSE results — they asked whether this is just another way of saying that intelligence is genetic. That is, are genetic studies of school results and of intelligence just measuring the same thing?

The answer is emphatically no. The authors found that a large chunk of the heritability of education is not explained by intelligence; in a complex statistical model, they showed that IQ score accounts for a quarter of the heritability of GCSE scores. Twenty-five per cent is a substantial chunk: indeed, IQ score makes the single largest contribution to the GCSE heritability of any trait measured in the study – and they measured a lot of traits.

Not including the two IQ measures, the twins filled in no fewer than 81 different questionnaires to tap educational self-efficacy, personality, well-being, behaviour problems, health, and school and home environment (bear in mind that most studies measure just one or two of these). These factors collectively accounted for a further 50 per cent of the heritability of education.

So we are at 75 per cent: three-quarters of the heritability of education can be explained by the heritabilities of a mix of all the factors above. Genes for education are, in large part, genes for intelligence, but also genes for personality, plus genes for motivation, plus the rest. So what accounts for the remaining quarter of the heritability? The authors don’t really speculate, but we might expect that even longer, more detailed questionnaires for each trait — as well as intelligence tests that cover more areas of cognition than the reasoning and vocabulary skills tested here — would do an even better job.

Because so many variables were studied, both the article itself and its online supplementary material section (both freely-downloadable at the PNAS website) are packed with information on the genetics of education: they should and will be used as a point of reference for years to come. The discussion towards the end on what our knowledge of genetics might imply for personalised learning (elaborated upon at more length by study co-authors Kathryn Asbury and Robert Plomin in their recent book G is for Genes) deserves to be read and debated by scientists, policymakers, and teachers.

Another view one often encounters when discussing genetics and education is this: “psychology is really complicated – isn’t everything a complex conjunction of genes and environments, too messy for science to disentangle?” Studies such as this one should dispel
this kind of pessimism. We can put reliable numbers on
the effects of genes versus environments on important
high-level outcomes such as education, and then begin to examine the underlying recipe of lower-level traits that explain them.

We most certainly don’t have all the answers yet, but large-scale, comprehensive analyses such as this one show that the answers are not outside our grasp.

 

More Reviews

Schools of thought: Lessons to learn from schools doing things differently

This book offers an enriching and varied tour of the best that schools around the globe have to offer

Find out more

The Conversation – with Zara Simpson

Ramadan, Women's History Month and British Science week

Find out more

The Conversation – with Dr Jess Mahdavi-Gladwell

The end of 'school phobia', inner leadership, belonging, curriculum and culture, and blank stares

Find out more

Teaching Classroom Controversies: Navigating Complex Teaching Issues in the Age of Fake News and Alternative Facts

This immediately practical and up-to-date book will help teachers navigate some of today's most contentious issues

Find out more

The Conversation – with Shekeila Scarlett

Pupils missing education, rethinking pastoral care, and homeschooling

Find out more

Your thoughts

Leave a Reply to PRice Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

One comment

  1. The study’s “genetic reasons” term doesn’t mean that the researchers actually took genetic samples. From one news article: “Identical twins share 100 percent of their genes while non-identical twins share just 50 percent of their genes. Because these sets of twins share the same environment, the scientists were able to compare identical and non-identical twins to estimate the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors.”

    This estimating method produced an artificial divide between genetic and environmental factors. Identical twins start out sharing 100% of their genes, but then their genes become expressed differently – often because of environmental factors – to produce unique individuals even before birth. The sets of identical twins were definitely not the 100% same genetic makeup between themselves at age 16 as they were at conception, and that assumption was the foundation of the estimating method.

    I feel that the researchers didn’t prove their case that “genetic reasons” were a causal factor to the stated extent. Although their estimating method’s numbers may have indicated that the above exercise was valid, that didn’t necessarily mean that the method’s results reflected the reality of genetic and epigenetic influences on the subjects. Better methods of estimating “the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors” are available with genetic sampling.

    The funniest thing I saw in the study’s news coverage was one where someone argued that the researchers were wrong and that they needed educational psychologists on their staff to interpret the data. Guess the profession of the arguer!

    http://surfaceyourrealself.com/2015/05/16/dividing-genetic-and-environmental-factors-when-assessing-educational-achievement-surfaceyourrealself/